|
|
Maya Götz
Children are enchanted, parents concerned
Research findings from Germany reflect
a worldwide trend: children have fun with the Teletubbies
and their parents have problems with this new programme format.
The Teletubbies are loved by children
throughout the world, but parents regard them with benign astonishment
or with outright disapproval. In Germany, at the latest since transmissions
began on 29th March 1999, the Teletubbies had
been part of the public discussion, and hardly any German newspaper
missed a chance of publishing a critical article on this series.
The monthly GfK data of the ARD/ZDF Children's Channel (Kinderkanal)
show that the series enjoys top ratings and makes the corporation
market leader during the day. And last not but not least, the –
at times empty – Teletubby shelves in the stores are proof
of the surprisingly great success, which – at least to this extent
– are a mystery, and not only for the experts.
What makes the children so enthusiastic about
this series, of all programmes? How do they make use of a programme
which is so redundant and lacking in content? How do parents assess
the Teletubbies reception by their children? What importance
for children attaches to the strange language or the homosexual
symbols that Jerry Falwell denounced?
To be able to offer some initial explanations
the IZI investigated these questions in a study. As reception research
is extremely difficult with this very young target group and the
programme format (at least in Germany) is new, a broader exploratory
phase was first necessary in which various proven methods were employed.
Subsequently parents' assessments and the reception situation in
the children's everyday life was examined.
- Exploratory phase1
Morning circle discussion in kindergarten and primary school (42
children);
Play and painting activities in the kindergarten (39 children);
Case studies in families with children who enjoy watching the
Teletubbies (5 families);
Evaluation of the viewers' letters and pictures to the Teletubby
Magazine (month of August);
Questionnaire sent to parents by Internet in the Forum of Family
& Co;
Sifting through 800 press reports in the months of February, March
and April 1999.
- Analysis of natural reception situations
of children between the ages of 1 and 62
Parents whose children enjoy watching the Teletubbies were
persuaded to take part. Using a video camera placed beside the
television set they filmed 2-3 "quite normal" Teletubbies
reception sessions per child. The parents were contacted through
the kindergarten and the Internet, especially over the Internet
Community Urbia.de. In this way 114 recordings of a very largely
natural reception situation of 40 children are available. The
Teletubbies instalment seen in each case is "stamped" into
the recordings as a small picture, so that it is possible to follow
both the child and the programme it saw.3
- Questionnaire sent to parents by Internet,
analysis of the AOL Chatforum.4
The IZI provided a questionnaire for the parents in altogether
11 Internet forums5 in the subject area of family,
parents and young children. The questions were deliberately left
open and were directed at three areas: experience with the Teletubbies
in everyday life, observation of children during Teletubbies
reception and assessment of the programme by the parents. 248
responses were received to this questionnaire.
The provider AOL set up a chat forum on
the subject of the Teletubbies on its opening page. Within
three days 346 statements were received here, which were evaluated
with regard to their positions and arguments.
Below, the results will be compiled to open
up perspectives on what makes the children so enthusiastic about
the Teletubbies, what positions parents tend to take on them
and which arguments they advance. In the light of the examples of
language and the depiction of gender, the differences in the importance
attached by parents and children become clearly and theoretically
understandable.
1. What makes children so enthusiastic about
the Teletubbies?
The characters and their movement
rituals
The characters are the focal point of the
children's fascination for the Teletubbies. This is what
they draw and name and what attracts their attention on first contact.
What makes the characters interesting is not only their unmistakable
colours and recognisable features, but above all their tubbiness
and their movements. The unusual movement patterns accompanied by
funny noises and the causes of their movements, which involve fixed
rituals, are mentioned and copied most frequently. When asked to
do so, children also talk about the Teletubbies' stories,
but here, compared with other children's programmes, they remain
extremely brief and always focus on the characters.6
The fascination is probably to be found less in the content of the
stories told than to a large extent in the characters themselves.
The Teletubbies and their Teletubbyland are cleverly
chosen and certainly form the basis for the programme's success.
In addition to the "high teddy potential" (Groebel, 1999) and a
physical appeal which makes you want to touch them (Brudny, 1999),
there must be something else very special about the Teletubbies
for them to be so successful and to gain whole groups of new viewers.7
The reception situation
The children are extremely active during
reception. They talk a lot, particularly when adults are present,
explain and comment on the scenes. Apart from language activities,
they also display movements with surprising frequency as a part
of the reception. The children wave, dance and jump about on the
sofa or lie down on the floor to wave their legs in the air.
Typical moments of the Teletubbies
reception are shown which are displayed to varying extents in almost
all the children in the study group in one of the recordings. On
the basis of short scenes in the video recordings these typical
moments are illustrated using the cases of Anne (3 years), Lara
(2½ years), Lars (3 years), Tim (4 years) and Tobias (6).
Explaining/noticing/commenting:
The children say something about
the content of the series.
Anne (3) is
very enthusiastic about the Teletubbies. When the programme
begins and she hears the opening bars, she rushes into the room
visibly excited: "My, my Teletubby, ouch, ouch, mine." She dashes
to her television chair and knocks into the table leg, but even
that cannot diminish her pleasure. She begins a sentence, then
breaks off, laughs at the baby and imitates his gestures. Anne
explains: "The baby in the sun, it's always laughing. Bab, ba,
ba, ba." She attentively follows the camera panning over the hills
in Teletubbyland and says, "Look, now, you'll see them
in a second."
The children talk during reception. Quite
often they notice something, comment on it and draw their parents'
attention to it. They frequently explain the connections to the
parents or other adults present, but sometimes they say it to themselves
when alone.
Speaking together/singing together:
Children speak or sing the text parallel to the programme.
Lara (2½) sings
and dances the entire opening sequence. After that she sits down
on the couch. When the bear and the lion finally come, she crooks
her hand in front of her face and joins in the passages: "I am
the bear, the bear am I, my fur is soft and fluffy." The more
often the bear says this, the more accurately she can speak the
words.
Many children join in speaking and singing
the recurring texts with enthusiasm and learn the rhymes and numbers
within a short time.
Following: Children watch as if
glued to the screen.
Tim (4 years)
sits in a large armchair, eating his supper. He is concentrating
hard on the action, smiles and waves to the Teletubbies.
When the film insert with the children comes, he looks at the
girls as if spellbound who are playing with balloons. Sometimes
he even forgets to carry on chewing. On one occasion he remains
with his mouth open for over 15 seconds.
This concentration could be observed in many
children while the film inserts were being shown. Whereas children
under 1½ years use these scenes for other activities, the older
ones seem to be spellbound. Especially films in which children actively
do something arouse their attention. Performances by adults (eg
Mrs Ulkig or Nena), however, are followed less intently. In the
repetition the concentration does not change and they watch the
stories in their entirety with the greatest interest for a second
time.
Answering: Children take up the
requests in the programme to answer and talk with the television
set.
The 'tummy tale' begins and two girls call
"Hallo". Anne (3 years) answers, "Hallo." The English girl comes
closer and again says, "Hallo" and Anne answers again, but this
time more softly and completely engrossed in the action, "Hallo."
The girl asks, "Do you want to see what we're doing?" and Anne replies,
"Yes." Once more she looks at her mother, then directs all her attention
to the television and, spellbound, follows the two children with
the dog. At the end the girls say, "Goodbye," and Anne answers in
a soft voice, "Goodbye."
Tim (4 years)
sits in his television armchair, quietly and attentively following
the programme. When Po waves straight into the camera and greets
the viewers with "Eh-oh!" he waves back with a loud "Eh-oh!".
In the series the Teletubbies and
in the films the children and the adults directly address the viewers.
This often happens and in a relatively predictable sequence. Usually
addressing the viewers is restricted to greeting and saying goodbye.
The children's articulated reply to these requests is probably part
of para-social interaction (Horten/Wohl, 1956), which is found here
in a ritualised context.
Predicting/anticipating: After
seeing only a few programmes children possess enough media knowledge
to be able to anticipate the communication forms and actions.
In the first
scene after the opening song the interior of the igloo can be
seen. The door opens, but no one is there. Anne (3 years) says,
"Here comes Dipsy, he's coming into the house." Laa Laa and Dipsy
enter the room. Anne explains, "Dipsy, and Laa Laa and Po and
Dipsy." The three Teletubbies come into the foreground
and greet the viewers with "Eh-oh!". Anne replies, "Eh-oh!" Tinky
Winky comes into the picture, stands beside the others and shows
them a Tubbytoast and says, "Here!", to which the others answer,
"Oh, there." Anne again realises what is happening and says, "Toasti."
Tinky Winky says, "Tubbytoast, Tubbytoast." Anne jumps up and
own in her chair enthusiastically and, turning to her mother,
calls out, "Tubbytoast, Tubbytoast!"
At the beginning of the scene Anne already
knows what will happen next. The door opens and at least one Teletubby
will come into the igloo. Soon after that this anticipation, although
in a different order, is confirmed by the series. A brief action
sequence follows, which Anne immediately understands and whose continuation
she can anticipate. She articulates this with the child's form of
'toast'. But when the figures again confirm her anticipation, she
is delighted and jumps up and down for joy.
Predicting and anticipating are built into
the narration of the programme. The development of the story is
extremely slow and predictable. The camera shots resemble one another.
If this is also a shortcoming of the series for media experts (cf
Hake in this journal), it does delight the children. After only
little viewing experience they develop the competence to anticipate
the scenes. They not only have to follow the scenes, but they can
already anticipate them.
Joining in dancing/moving: On the
basis of the fixed sequences and the catchy music, children develop
their own choreography or move freely to the music.
Lara (2½) sits on the sofa
with a slice of cheese in her hand. The beginning of the series,
which she speaks out, can be heard from the television. When the
rhythm of the opening music sounds and the Teletubbies
run down the hill, Laura jumps up. After glancing at her mother
and father, sitting beside her at the table, she tap dances in
time to the music, with her forearms bent and fingers spread out
and pointing towards the television. She turns round on her own
axis and joins in singing the song. When the Teletubbies
run around, she takes four steps backwards and forwards in front
of the television. When the Teletubbies make their individual
appearances she imitates the main movement pattern in each case.
When the figures run away, she quickly runs round in a circle.
The sentence is heard "Where have the Teletubbies gone?",
and Lara, repeating the question, runs back to the sofa. She sits
there quietly, imitating especially the sun and repeating particular
parts. When the windmill turns to select a Teletubby for the tummy
tale, she jumps down from the sofa, runs in front of the television
and lies down on the ground like the Teletubbies to kick
her legs in the air. Then she stands up, her arms akimbo, and
pushes out her tummy, while she speaks the names of the figures.
She runs back to the sofa, where she follows the stories with
interest. When at the end the speaking tube comes up out of the
ground, Lara again stands in front of the television, waves to
the individual Teletubbies and then briefly bends her knees,
as if hiding behind an imaginary hill. The Teletubbies
jump into their hill, and Lara hops in the same manner. This sequence
is repeated next day, although with a doll, and on the following
day with a soft toy from 101 Dalmatians in her hand. Her mother
says that she keeps this up for months on end.
Unlike what has been perceived hitherto in
the discussion on the subject of television and children, movement
is a frequently occurring part of the Teletubbies reception
– almost always in the form of arm movements (eg waving), often
as swaying from side to side to the music, and partly in dances
and jumps. For Lara it has become a fixed ritual, like the singing
and dancing games that she will love to join in at kindergarten
later on. What is most surprising here is the diversity of movements
displayed, as well as the fixed patterns she has developed herself
in imitation of the Teletubby figures.
Asking questions: At some points
children ask themselves about deeper connections which they would
like their parents to explain.
Lars (3 years) is sitting
on the sofa watching Teletubbies, while his parents are
having breakfast. Lars turns to the breakfast table: "Daddy, why
don't the Teletubbies have a penis?" His father is somewhat
taken aback: "What? Well, er, because they're wearing clothes."
Lars replies, "No." His father thinks for a moment, and then he
offers another explanation, "Well, because they're not proper
little men." Though Lars is not really satisfied with this answer,
he turns back to the programme.
Children are moved by anything they cannot
solve for themselves. In many sections the programme is self-explanatory.
According to the descriptions by the parents and in natural reception
situations, in the case of the Teletubbies there are relatively
few questions, except in the animated cartoon Animal Parade,
when the children often ask the names of certain animals (eg flamingo).
Games during reception: The programme
does not necessarily have to be the focus of attention the whole
time. At some points the children run out or occupy themselves intensively
with something else.
Anne (3 years) is standing
right in front of the television holding a Po doll in her hand.
She addresses the Teletubbies directly: "Here is a Po as
well. I also have a Po. Look, Teletubbies (loud), look, look,
look." She repeats this several times in the next five minutes,
saying it louder and louder. Anne has one concern: she wants to
show her new doll to the Teletubbies. She only follows the action
on the screen inasmuch as she addresses the Teletubbie standing
in the foreground each time. When Tinky Winky, without hearing
her request, turns away she says, "I'll try later," and waits
until another of the characters turns his front to her. A feeling
of uncertainty because the Teletubbies do not answer her
directly cannot be noticed, at least not in her face. Anne is
engrossed in her play world.
In several reception situations the children
start playing. Here with a doll in front of the television, sometimes
with children present.
Tobias (6 years) and his
sister (1 year) regularly watch the Teletubbies. The two
often nudge each other gently with their noses and roll over the
sofa together. Occasionally both attentively follow the programme.
Then Tobias fetches out his Saban plastic figures and plays with
them: "I'll fight, and you get angry. Quick! You have to rescue
yourself." He briefly looks at the screen again and explains the
connections to his mother. Then he is again absorbed in his game
on and under the sofa. When his mother realises what he is doing
she asks whether she should switch off the television, and Tobias
replies very firmly that she should not.
Tobias and his sister use the programme for,
among other things, playing together. The Teletubbies becomes
an accompanying medium for some time and seems to be attractive,
especially for older children, functioning much like a radio play.
Several children divert their attention away from the television
for a time, play at something else or even leave the room for a
short time.
The narrative structure with built-in
learning forms
Structure of the Programme |
Leader |
Short
story |
Selection
procedure |
Tummy story with repetition
|
Long story in 'Teletubbyland'
|
Dances/animations |
Saying
goodbye |
Figure 1:
|
The individual Teletubbies programmes
follow a relatively fixed structure of eight elements (cf Fig 1).
The elements 6 and 7, however, can only be found in some Teletubbies
instalments. Nearly a quarter of the programme is almost identical
from one broadcast to the next.
- Element 1: Leader
(2 mins 30 secs)
The sun with the baby in it rises and the Teletubbies introduce
themselves with their distinctive Teletubby song.
- Element 2: Short story in Teletubbyland
(1 min on average)
In the bunker or in the hilly landscape the Teletubbies
play little games with one another, with the rhymes that come
out of the speaking tubes, with favourite objects or the equipment
in the igloo.
- Element 3:Selection procedure
(1 min 25 secs)
Using a constantly recurring procedure the Teletubby is selected
in whose tummy the film is to be seen afterwards with the children.
- Element 4: Tummy tales
(2 x about 3 mins 30 secs)
The films show children in everyday life aged between 3 and 6
years. Experiences from the area of the home, a visit to the parents'
workplace, making things from paper etc, experiences with animals
and nature or short films are shown in which adults read to children
or sing with them. When the children say goodbye for the first
time, the four Teletubbies in Teletubbyland call "no-mal"
(= noch einmal = again) until the film has been completely repeated.
- Element 5: Long story in Teletubbyland
(average 5 mins)
Stories are told introduced by a male voice with the words "One
day in Teletubbyland ...". Funny events result from everyday occurrences
such as the wish for a Tubbypudding or playing outdoors, eg flying
kites.
- Element 6: Tubbydances
(about 2 mins)
After simple and catchy music in four-four time the four Teletubbies
dance altogether eight different dances.
- Element 7: Performances in Teletubbyland
(about 2 mins 30 secs/bear and lion about 4 mins)
The Teletubbies gather to follow one of the six different
computer animations or a game of hide-and-seek by the bear and
lion.
- Element 8: Saying goodbye (
2 mins 30 secs)
The four Teletubbies say goodbye twice to the viewers and
finally jump into their house hill after waving again. The baby
sun sets.
- Intermediate elements: Sun and pan
across the hills
(up to 10 secs)
Within and between the individual elements the face of a baby
appears in the computer-animated sun.
During reception the Tubbydances,
the constantly recurring Teletubby scenes in the elements
1, 3 and 8 or the intermediate elements are not followed with great
concentration. In contrast to that, the children – at least the
older ones – often stare at the screen as if spellbound during the
tummy tales. In this case they usually sit quietly, whereas they
frequently move during other elements. There is a connection between
the typical moments of reception and the elements of the programme
(cf Fig 2). This finding at first appears quite trivial, but is
of basic importance for an understanding of the Teletubbies
and in the end also for their success. The different elements suggest
different use forms within the programme.
Hitherto it has been assumed that the ideal
reception behaviour – if it has to be – should be that the child
follows the story being told with concentration and fascination,
ie that it watches, understands and, if possible, remembers a programme.
To catch the child's eye and hold its attention, colours, tones
and the rhythms of the cuts etc are adapted to children's perception
and ability to pay attention. In learning-oriented programmes a
presenter takes over the guidance and explains to the viewers the
connections in the world. In the area of fiction certain figures,
the dramatisation and camera angles take over the function of explaining.
Here the young recipients are given a relatively fixed position
in which they are guided and taught by people acting in an educationally
responsible way.
Typical moments in the reception situation |
Leader |
Speaking/singing/dancing
with the Teletubbies. Predicting/anticipating,
answering |
Selection
procedure |
Saying
goodbye |
Tummy
tale |
Following,
answering (welcoming), noticing/with repetition recognising |
Short
story |
Following, predicting/anticipating, noticing/explaining/commenting,
speaking along with the Teletubbies |
Long
story in Teletubbyland |
Dances/animations |
Dancing with
the Teletubbies/moving/anticipating, other play,
asking questions, noticing (Animal Parade), joining in
speaking (bear/lion) |
Figure 2
|
The Teletubbies offer something new
here. What is shown is simplified and abstracted. The camera angle
leaves plenty of room for the viewer and is not restricted by a
commentary to focus attention. The calm rhythm of the cuts gives
the viewer time to think about what has been seen, to follow it
again and even, depending on the motivation and individual theme,
to overtake the action. This makes it possible to experience competence
and activates communication forms such as have not been possible
hitherto in programmes with an educational intention, at least not
with this consistency. In some elements, especially in the intermediate
elements and the Tubbydances, the programme even turns the
recipients away from the television, "the picture-magnet" (Böhme-Dürr,
1999). The children avert their gaze, play at something else or
move around. What at first sight looks like a scripting mishap is
a welcome break for the children. Precisely because the Teletubbies
call for something different from the viewers than just following
the continuing action, they are especially interesting for young
children. Many parents are surprised at the long attention span
and the emotional bonding of the children. It arises because the
viewers are repeatedly addressed directly by figures in cleverly
chosen colours that are physically attractive with interesting movements,
and the structure of the programme again and again offers them different
areas of reception. The Teletubbies is a magazine for the
young viewers. It does not, however, consist, as is usual in children's
television, of different stories which have to be followed, but
of a collection of reception areas, which suggest different learning
forms. Here the four Teletubbies form the links. They are
not only the protagonists, but a framework for the other elements
within the programme. Thus the viewers together with the Teletubbies
watch the tummy tales or the performances in Teletubbyland.
By all the elements being linked up with the Teletubbies in one
form or another, the enjoyment of the reception situation is also
transferred to the merchandising articles and thus accounts, among
other things, for the enormous demand.
2. The concern of parents
and adults
In view of this diversity of activities,
the programme seems to be harmless, even promoting competence. Nevertheless,
adults are divided in their opinion of the programme.
The positions of the parents
IZI's Internet
questionnaire attracted 248 responses. The age of the children whose
parents participated was chiefly between 1½ and 3½ years. Even though
the parents who were reached are certainly not statistically representative,
it is possible to obtain an insight into their attitude. It can
be divided into three positions:
- The first
group is made up of parents, almost exclusively mothers, who reject
the programme. They frequently argue that sitting in front of
the television set is no place for young children anyway, but
when they do view they should see children's television "classics"
such as Die Sendung mit der Maus or Sesame Street.
These, they believe, best stimulate their children, and they report
on the pleasure they themselves had and have from these programmes.
- A second,
smaller group regards the Teletubbies critically and with
a certain lack of understanding. These parents notice a difference
between their own and their children's taste, and "in spite of
this" they mostly allow their children to see the programme against
this background.
- The third
group thinks the Teletubbies is good and describes it as
suitable for children, even more so than Sesame Street
for television beginners. These parents greatly enjoy watching
the programmes with their children.
In the AOL Chatforum
it was also possible to discern this division into three groups.
If we may generalise about the two random samples even only very
cautiously, they do at least indicate that adults' opinions in Germany
on the subject of the Teletubbies is divided. Arguments for
this are not only fundamental ideas about what education should
be, but also certain aspects of the programme. In the reasons given
for the different assessments specific factors of the programme
– like the special Teletubbies' use of language, the redundant
narrative structure or something that can be paraphrased as "educational
content" – are given as pros or cons. Table 1 shows the hit parade
of the parents' arguments for or against the Teletubbies.
The argument
advanced most frequently by the parents taking part in the IZI questionnaire
for the Teletubbies is the children's observation
during reception. The most frequently given argument against
the Teletubbies is that sitting in front of the television
is no place for children at this age. In this context "parking the
children", ie television in a baby-sitting function, is mentioned
and rejected. Educationally committed positions are set against
pragmatic considerations of organising the household with several
children. On a deeper level, the contributions are therefore often
concerned about the definition of "a good mother" and about the
extent to which television and in particular the Teletubbies
can be reconciled with this idea.
Typically the
parents who reject the Teletubbies argue on the basis of
relatively firm ideas about educational content and how it is acquired.
The parents with a positive attitude to the Teletubbies argued
by attempting to understand their children's perspectives.
IZI Questionnaire, 248 responses by parents:
85 positive, 70 negative, 32 undecided,
61 not parents or not serious |
|
AOL Chatforum, 346 responses by adults:
109 positive, 132 negative,
34 undecided, 71 not serious |
Positive |
Negative |
|
Positive |
Negative |
The children
concentrate during reception, are active, think and join in.
(20) |
In front of
the television is no place for children (30) |
1. |
The Teletubbies
are free of violence. (33) They are more suitable than other
children's programmes, here the Power Rangers are often
mentioned. (33) |
The used language
is unsuitable and results in language problems. (50) |
The Teletubbies
are suitable for children, in some cases even more so than other
children's programmes. Sesame Street often mentioned.
(18) |
The language
used is unsuitable and might result in language problems. (26) |
2. |
The language
used is very suitable and is not dangerous for language development.
(23) |
The Teletubbies
dulls the children's minds. (47) |
The language
used is very suitable and not dangerous, or even supports language
development. (14) |
The Teletubbies
do not take children seriously and are too simple. (12) |
3. |
The Teletubbies
are fun and entertainment for the children without an instructive
background. (11) |
Sitting in
front of the television is no place for small children. (28) |
The programme
is free of violence. (12) |
The Teletubbies
are merely a way to market children. (11) |
4. |
The Teletubbies
are instructive and educationally valuable. (10) |
The programme
is not instructive and has no educational content. (21) |
Accordingly it can altogether be said: the
position of parents towards the programme Teletubbies is
closely connected with the general idea of how to deal with children.
Here the respective perceptions and assessments of childhood and
society are projected into the programme.
The adults' fear of language degeneration
An argument that is repeatedly mentioned
is the language used in the programme and its potential significance
for the children. While some fear retrogression in language development,
other parents deem it to be suitable for children and supportive
of language. Both assessments interestingly relate exclusively to
the language used by the Teletubby characters. The employment
of language in the programme takes place, however, on three levels:
- Adults' voices of serve as a setting for
the programme. The speaker (Fabian Harloff) introduces the scene,
speaks with the Teletubbies and describes the story. Songs
and rhymes can be heard coming from the speaking tubes and they
are presented by adults. Altogether they are simple short sentences
spoken in standard German.
- In the tummy tales the children are the
focal point of the action. They speak as they move around, comment
off screen on the scenes, using language in keeping with their
current vocabulary and ability to articulate.
- The Teletubby characters employ
language in their own form. Besides the Teletubby expressions
(Eh-oh, Tubbytoast, Tubbypudding etc) the Teletubbies use a special
language. Firstly, there are language fragments to express their
own feelings (Oh, oh, Oh, no, Laalila, Peidi-Peidi-Po etc). Secondly,
words ("No mal" instead of "Noch mal" = again) and sentence constructions
are shortened. Occasionally the Teletubbies speak short sentences
in their entirety ("What is that?"), usually they simply leave
out verbs and articles ("Ball back", instead of "The ball is back").
The Laa Laa also often attaches an "i" to the end of a word, and
Po sometimes does not speak certain consonants.
The language used in the programme tends
to be a game with language, in which various levels are interconnected.
Parents notice the special way in which the Teletubbies talk,
and many of them fear negative consequences for their children's
language development. The basic assumption here is that the language
used by the Teletubbies – and not that of the speaker or
the children in the tummy tales – has a direct effect on language
learning. They see their fears confirmed by what they observe in
their children:
"My daughter's entire kindergarten
is meanwhile in a state of language disintegration. Soon no child
will be able to say 'hallo' any longer. Now I hear only "Eh-oh,
Eh-oh" almost all the time." (Ilona 2nd July 99)
"My children (5 and 3 years
old) love the Tubbies, but they quickly began to drop to
their language level, whereupon the programme was immediately
cut out of their television viewing." (Petra 30th June
99)
Others, for the most part undecided parents,
emphasise the importance of the parents as an authority for corrections.
"Sanja (3 years) adores
watching Teletubbies. (...) What really disturbs me is
the simplified language. I have to constantly correct my daughter
when she starts saying "Eh-oh" or "No mal, no mal" (for German
noch mal) after watching a Teletubbies instalment."
(Kirstn 2nd August 99)
The parents' observations all agree that
children adopt certain expressions from the programme and use them
in everyday life. This perception is interpreted differently and
results in different educational measures.
"Eh-oh, hallo and Grüß-Gott"
– What children take over from the language of the Teletubbies into
their everyday life.
In the morning circle discussion 3- 6-year-old
children are also talking about the way he Teletubbies express
themselves.
Example:
Interviewer: Mm. They
sometimes have a language of their own, don't they?
Julian: Eh-oh means
hallo.
Interv: Eh-oh means
hallo. What sort of things do they say then?
Torben: Bye bye means,
er, means goodbye. And hallo, um, Eh-oh means hallo.
Children know the special way the Teletubbies
express themselves and translate it into standard German: "Eh-oh
means hallo." They also discuss the meaning in some cases, for example,
the "Oh, oh", the sound the Teletubbies use to express surprise.
This indicates that children do not confuse these expressions with
their own language, but learn them like new words in their vocabulary.
Laura, 2½ years old, from Ingolstadt is an
enthusiastic Teletubbies viewer (see above). In the video
tapings of the reception situations she always sings the opening
song. This ends with the words "The Teletubbies say hallo.
(Teletubbies reply to the viewer:) Eh-oh." Lara sings, "Eh-oh,
hallo and Grüss Gott" (a southern German greeting). Lara uses
three different variants for greeting. According to her mother "Grüss
Gott" is Lara's only Bavarian expression, as the family comes from
Thuringia (in eastern Germany). Besides the greeting she already
knows: "Hallo" (standard German), she also uses two new ones she
has learnt: "Eh-oh" (Teletubby greeting) and "Grüss Gott".
Learning these words is probably closely
connected with the redundant narrative form of the programme, the
body-language of the figures and the children's previous knowledge.
The children follow the story, interpret the non-verbal language
and associate this with the new words. Very soon they correctly
guess the meaning of the Teletubby vocabulary and explain
it. It is also possible to learn foreign expressions in this way.
Quite a few letters were received in which the positive importance
of the Teletubbies was mentioned for children growing up
with several languages. In one family with four children which has
to spend several months in Spain because of the parents' job the
Teletubbies are used to learn the first Spanish words.
The result was three young children who
started to speak Spanish within two days, eg "Is that my pelota?"
(Cornelia 29th August 1999)
Children acquire the Teletubbies'
expressions and use them in everyday life. The expressions remind
parents of an earlier development stage, so that they interpret
this media behaviour (cf Paus-Haase, 1998) as 'baby-language', ie
language that is not yet properly developed. A number of parents
and educators see their educational endeavours thwarted. Other parents
see no problem in this, describe it as a short-term phase or even
think it helps their children.
The findings would indicate that the Teletubbies
do not result in retrogressive language development, but in an expansion
of the language repertoire. As these words are at variance with
many adults' concept of a 'proper use of language', the children,
by integrating Teletubby expressions, circumvent the prevalent
idea of how children should progress in their language development.
Learning the Teletubbies' expressions is therefore principally
a problem for adults, who see their power of definition with regard
to the words used by the child undermined.8 As public
service television in particular makes an effort to consider parents'
ideas about children's television, the Teletubbies certainly
marks a turning point. The argument that a programme should be offered
that is especially attractive to children is here just as understandable
as the feelings of some parents with firm ideas about education
that the ARD/ZDF Children's Channel is no longer on their side.
How do the children use the Teletubbies
language?
When children learn Teletubby words
as additions to their vocabulary it has to be asked for what purpose
they employ these words. Here are some examples from the study:
- A typical expression as the reason
for a game
Kay and Chantal both refer to themselves
in free play as Laa Laa. At first they play with two other children
with a large ball. When the two other children turn aside, Kay
says, "Come on, now we'll say, Laa Laa play with ball". Then both:
"Laa Laa play with ball." They push the ball and run after it,
squealing. A role game ensues in which movement is the central
motive.
In free play the switch into the Teletubbies'
language becomes the reason for the game and a typical phrase from
the programme becomes the framework for a movement-oriented game.
- The Teletubbies' language as a secret
language
In the third form of a reformed school
the language of the Teletubbies, or an interpretation of
it, is becoming a secret language. In the breaks the four boys,
who are firm friends, talk in a kind of baby-language which they
call Teletubby. The four boys' parents are getting worked
up about this, which, however, only welds the friends closer together.
If girls or female teachers come to them in the break, they say
nothing, and just giggle when the outsiders go away.
The media relation becomes a joke in the
break, the secret language being used to form a group and draw a
clear line between them and the others.
The Teletubby way of talking as a defence
mechanism
In one case a mother reports how the Teletubbies'
language has a negative effect on her 5-year-old son's ability
to pronounce words. The boy is undergoing speech therapy. In the
summer holidays he watches the programme with his sister, and
his mother is aware of changes: "The words he speaks properly
suddenly turned out to be wrong. (...) Now that he goes to day-care
centre Andreas talks sensibly again. THANK HEAVENS! (...)" (Bettina
21st August 1999). When asked directly, the mother
explains the connections: "Andreas got worse by parroting the
Teletubbies. They don't talk properly either, and extremely indistinctly.
Andreas imitated that." (Bettina 23rd August 1999)
A 5-year-old undergoing speech therapy "parroted"
the Teletubbies' language in the summer holidays. The mother's problems
are understandable. Probably everyday life in the family is characterised
by efforts to encourage the boy to speak clearly, for which purpose
they are also availing themselves of professional help. After Andreas
saw the Teletubbies it is quite conceivable that he was thankful
to adopt the Teletubbies' words into his vocabulary as they
are easier to pronounce. Probably Andreas used the Teletubbies
as a kind of den into which he could retreat to escape his mother's
efforts (especially in the summer holidays).
Children employed the Teletubbies'
expressions in a subjective sense. The phrases turn, for example,
into the reason for a game, a secret language or a defence mechanism.
The game with gender clichés
The perception and assessment of gender presentation
are not so directly aligned to educational concepts as to their
own experiences. In the official version, the two larger Teletubbies
Tinky Winky and Dipsy are boy-figures and Laa Laa and Po girl-figures.
Unlike in the usual children's programmes, the girl-figures are
not distinguished by any special features, such as bows in their
hair or sexualised features. This does not mean, however, that they
have no gender or are not based on gender rituals. Gitta Mühlen
Achs, referring to Goffman, proves the special significance of body-language
in media productions (cf Mühlen Achs, 1998, 1995). Dipsy and
Laa Laa, in particular, adopt an existing ritualisation to indicate
gender. Dipsy often stands with legs apart and displays expressively
energetic gestures. Laa Laa, on the other hand, presents herself
with ballet-like movements and a body pose which reveals her as
a "typical girl". Po's movements, however, are far less graceful.
She often stands with her legs apart or hops up and down. Tinky
Winky gesticulates less than the other Teletubbies and his
movements are somewhat clumsy. The favourite objects of the three
smaller Teletubbies (Dipsy: hat, Laa Laa: ball, Po: scooter)
are only partly gender-typical. Tinky Winky's red lady's handbag,
on the other hand, is a "feminine" accessory. Whereas the figures
Laa Laa and Dipsy correspond more to the existing stereotypes of
girls and boys, in the case of Tinky Winky and Po the prevalent
gender clichés are extended by play.
In the reception this leads to an astonishing
development. For adults the figure Tinky Winky with his red handbag
is confusing. Not only the Reverend Falwell, but also a number of
mothers in the questionnaire feel rather disturbed by this combination.
The idea that a member of the dominant group (men) would voluntarily
acquire an accessory of the subdominant group (women) does not fit
in with their interpretation pattern, unless in the context of a
deviant "masculinity" (subdominant group men with homosexual orientation).
For children Tinky Winky is clearly a boy.
They also agree in the case of Laa Laa, whom they all describe as
a girl. For children it is Po whose sex is unclear. Almost all the
children talk about her in play and stories in the masculine form.
If older children and primary school children are asked directly
about this figure, they state that Po is a girl. A little later,
when retelling stories, in exceptionally gripping instalments they
again talk about her in the masculine form. Even girls in the 3rd
form refer to her as 'he', as do the children who wrote letters
to the magazine.9
Probably the use of the masculine form partly
results from the gender-specifically neutral name Po and body-language
that is unusual for media presentations. Television-experienced
children are, moreover, used to all figures regarded as positive
being naturally male, unless they bear obvious "feminine" features.
In the appropriation of the figures in the
play-activities further exciting moments emerge. After the children
have built a Teletubbyland with cushions, they play freely
with the Teletubby dolls. After that comes the request to
be Teletubbies themselves. The girls want to be Laa Laa above
all, but also Po, Tinky Winky and, in one case, Dipsy. The boys'
first choice is Po, sometimes Tinky Winky and Laa Laa, but Dipsy
in only a few cases. In everyday life, too, the children's interest
is not directed automatically to the figure of the same sex. Thus
Anne (3) finds Tinky Winky the most interesting. The theme that
guides her actions is to be tall and to prove her competency. An
Italian boy loves Po most of all – Ricardo (3¼) lovingly feeds his
Laa Laa doll.
Up to now the idea of two separate worlds
has dominated reception research: one girls' media world focusing
on harmony and common interests and a boys' media world specialising
in fighting and separation (cf, eg, Paus-Haase, 1998, p 9). Those
responsible for making programmes assume that for girls appropriation
processes are possible, above all with the few girl and women figures
and under certain conditions with the boy and men figures. For boys,
on the other hand, it is taken for granted that the female figures
offer them practically nothing.10 In view of the stereotype
role distribution on children's television this is understandable,
but this is also a self-fulfilling prophecy. In children's everyday
life the variants in experience with women and men have multiplied.
The puzzling gender, the playful handling of sexual clichés,
is nowadays already part of childhood. A mother's thoughts on this:
"(...) Anyone observing children sees that
boys enjoy wearing make-up like their mothers and girls shaving
their faces like their fathers, so Tinky-Winky carries a red handbag.
My son and my daughter quarrel about which of them may carry my
handbag. So what? That's normal." (K. Rykowski)
The Teletubbies as part of the individual
formation of meaning
The reception and interpretation of the programme
is part of the media appropriation and thus part of the individual
formation of meaning. The perceptions, interpretations and actions
with the media are permeated by individual themes, social context,
biography and the specific medium in their intertextual involvement
in the media market (Bachmair, 1996).
Parents see a medium which deviates in many
ways from what they have experienced and know for themselves. They
themselves grew up with the programmes Die Sendung mit
der Maus and Sesame Street and experienced them as happy
and harmless. In the public discussion, the subject of children
and television was and is problematised, and warnings were and are
given about damaging consequences. As the parents were able to experience
the "classics" themselves and ascertain for themselves – often even
contrary to the public debate - that they were harmless, it is possible
for them to maintain their own standpoint here.
On the television market the Teletubbies
is an unusual narrative form which can hardly be grasped by the
previous understanding of an educationally intended, entertaining
or exciting children's programme. Added to this is the expansion
of the target group and the violation of a taboo associated with
it on German television. Here the Teletubbies touches on
deeper fears and attacks the "last safe bastion, early childhood"
(cf Howard & Roberts in this journal). The press and commercial
channels use the opportunity and report on the programme in the
usual emotion-laden and deliberately critical style.11
The private commercial channel SAT.1 even transmitted a page of
teletext reporting on the growth in speech-therapy made necessary
on account of Teletubbies consumption. Here people's potential
fears are being exploited. (After all, the press and SAT.1 are mainly
interested in keeping and gaining viewers.)
Parents would like to give their children
the best possible start in life. What parents understand by this
varies from individual to individual. On account of their own (television)
experiences, they themselves find it difficult to properly assess
the Teletubbies and seek guidance. They experience the public
discussion principally as critical reporting.
In the reception situation parents experience
their children gripped by a hitherto unknown degree of fascination.
They use their previous interpretation patterns and describe their
children's Teletubby-fascination using, for example, words
such as drug addiction or hypnosis.
For pre-school children, some of whom are
experiencing television for the first time with the Teletubbies,
the problems of interpretation do not even arise. They take for
granted the play with language and the gender presentation. The
aesthetic design or the redundant dramatisation is often not a subject
that concerns children, unless on first contact (cf Best in this
journal). With this series their interpretation patterns, their
aesthetic and narrative experience first come into existence, because
a large number of children are beginning their television viewing
time with the Teletubbies. By moving the time slot to the
evening, the Teletubbies and the Sandmännchen
will become for many the only thing they are regularly allowed to
watch. So this pre-school programme will leave its mark, at least
in Germany, on the television experiences of a whole generation
of infants – certainly with far-reaching consequences.
The
Teletubbies:
a
children's programme
that
touches on the nerve of time
The Teletubbies touches on the nerve
of time. They consistently touch on the children's "nerve" educationally
and commercially with sensitivity, intuition and a little bit of
happiness. But they also touch on the exposed nerves of the times.
For parents it is not only the burden of having to deal with the
subject of television and opportunities for consumption at an ever
earlier age of their children, but what they chiefly lack is interpretation
patterns to be able to understand and judge this programme format.
At the latest with the Teletubbies it becomes clear to many
that they can no longer naturally understand children's culture
on the basis of their own childhood experience. This realisation
would have come anyway as the children grew older, but now the first
three years of life, which have become a myth, are also under attack.
Other media take up these fears and – also only economically consistent
– make their profits from more or less well researched stories.
To want to judge the Teletubbies in
this complex web as good or bad always means evaluating the "nerve
of time" as well. This television series is an innovation which
involved risks for all concerned. It has succeeded, has brought
in a lot of money for Ragdoll Productions, BBC-Worldwide, Itsy-Bitsy
Entertainment etc and confronted families and kindergartens with
the same problem: they have to find a way of interpreting, classifying
and coping with the subject of the Teletubbies.
NOTES:
1The play and
painting activities in the kindergarten were initiated and carried
out by Bärbel Kopp. The morning circle discussions and case
studies in families took place in cooperation with Ole Hofmann,
a graduate economist, who also analysed the contents of the 90
Teletubbies instalments in the first series.
2Carried out in cooperation with Ole Hoffmann, a graduate
economist.
3In a certain way this setting recalls experimental
research into media effects. The evaluation of this study takes
place, however, against the background of a theoretical approach
to qualitative reception research based on everyday life (cf Bachmair,
1996, Mikos, 1994). Unlike the Australian study by Susan Howard
and Susan Roberts (see announcement in this journal), the children
see different consequences of the first series in their everyday
life. For that reason alone, the main point of emphasis is not
on the "effect" of individual aspects of a certain consequence,
but is placed on typical moments in the Teletubbies reception
in general.
4Carried out with the support of Maria Monninger, Dieter
Grassberger, Bärbel Kopp and Wolfgang Vogt.
5Urbia.de, eltern.de, familie-online.de, family.acw.at,
elternnetz.de, elternwelt.de, kidnet.de, hausfrauenseite.de, babyzimmer.de,
rund-ums-baby.de
6In the morning circle discussion in kindergarten and
primary school, a basket containing various merchandising products
provides the opportunity to tell a story. Besides the Teletubbies
there is Mouse and Elephant from the Sendung mit der Maus and
Tiger and Piggy from Winnie the Pooh, Ernie and Bert from Sesame
Street and Rudi the Raven from Siebenstein etc. The children are
asked to take a figure out of the basket and to talk about the
programme and the stories.
7In the questionnaire to the parents it becomes clear
that the programme is turned on not only by children used to television,
but also introduces many children to deliberate viewing who hitherto
have only looked on.
8In other programmes, like Sesame Street, this problem
does not arise. Here, too, words are taken over into everyday
life. These include, in addition to Ernie's laugh, the numbers
1-30 or the sounds of the letters. This is in keeping with the
parents' dominant idea of a sensible learning content. Primary
school teachers have been complaining for many years about "children
ruined by Sesame Street". For the first reading lessons calling
the consonant "F" "ef" is a hindrance, and the children have to
be told to be careful about this previous knowledge. If reading
is still learnt from individual letters, then the letter "F" is
given its sound value. The terms the children learn for the letters
from Sesame Street is, however, for the prevalent idea unproblematic.
For that reason parents, unlike primary school teachers, see no
problem here, for the parents' power of definition is not questioned.
9In the letters many female writers refer to Po in
the masculine form. Here a striking similarity emerges to fan
letters written by 9- to 10-year-old girls to boy groups, indicating
forms of para-social relations (cf Götz, 1999).
10Albert Schäfer talks of an iron law of children's
television: "In productions with heroines one always runs the
risk of keeping the boys away from the television set" (quoted
by Gangloff, 1999)
11One of the few exceptions to this is the journal
Familie & Co, which tries to make parents understand their
children's enthusiasm for the programme. (Familie & Co, No 6/1999)
REFERENCES
- Bachmair, Ben: Fernsehkultur. Subjektivität
in einer Welt bewegter Bilder. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag 1996,
357 S.
- Böhme-Dürr, Karin: Bildmagnet Fernsehen. In: TelevIZIon,12/1999/1,
S. 19-24.
- Brudny, Wolfgang: Die "Teletubbies". Verunsichern
sie die Medienpädagogen? In: TV-Diskurs, -/1999/Oktober,
S. 67-73.
- Gangloff, Tilmann: Lauter Superhelden und Sexbomben. Schon in
den Sendungen des Kinderfernsehens dominieren die Geschlechterklischees.
In: Stuttgarter Zeitung vom 17.09.1999.
- Götz, Maya: Mädchen und Fernsehen. Facetten der Medienaneignung
in der weiblichen Adoleszenz. München: KoPäd 1999, 400
S.
- Groebel, Jo: Stimmungsfernsehen, Wohlfühlfernsehen, ohne
Angst vor Überzuckerung. Kinderkanal ARD/ZDF Pressemappe
zu den Teletubbies 1999.
- Horten, Donald; Wohl, Richard: Mass communication and para-social
interaction. Observations on intimacy at the distance. In: Psychiatry,
19/1956/3, S. 215-229.
- Mikos, Lothar: Es wird dein Leben! Familienserien im Fernsehen
und im Alltag der Zuschauer. Münster: MAkS Publikationen
1994, 475 S.
- Mühlen Achs, Gitta: Frauenbilder. Konstruktionen des anderen
Geschlechts. In: Mühlen Achs, Gitta ; Schorb, Bernd (Hrsg.):
Geschlecht und Medien. München: KoPäd 1995.
- Mühlen Achs, Gitta: Geschlecht bewußt gemacht. Körpersprachliche
Inszenierungen. München: Frauenoffensive 1998, 142 S.
- Paus-Haase, Ingrid: Heldenbilder im Fernsehen. Eine Untersuchung
zur Symbolik von Fernsehfavoriten. Opladen u.a.: Westdeutscher
Verlag 1998, 322 S.
AUTHOR
Maya Götz, Ph.D., is an academic member
of the staff of the IZI, Munich.
maya-goetz@brnet.de
www.maya-goetz.de
INFORMATION
Internationales
Zentralinstitut
für das Jugend-
und Bildungsfernsehen
IZI
Tel.: +49 89 - 59 00 21 40
Fax.: +49 89 - 59 00 23 79
eMail: izi@brnet.de
COPYRIGHT
© Internationales Zentralinstitut für
das Jugend- und Bildungsfernsehen (IZI) 2001
|
 |